Teaching Science Communication

Experiences from Psychology

  • Stefanie Peykarjou (Author)

Identifiers (Article)

Abstract

Communicating scientific findings to the general public is an important part of universities’ third mission. While the need for communicating research is amounting, university graduates hardly receive any training and are thus not well prepared to meet this challenge, be it as a researcher (e. g. during a PhD) or in the applied field of their studies. To address this gap, I have incorporated science communication training into my teaching and developed dedicated science communication courses for Psychology students. This paper focuses on a course on “Communicating Findings from Developmental Psychology” at Masters’ level, which I taught at Heidelberg University during the summer semester of 2022, and is replenished with experiences from a general course on “Science Communication” at Bachelor’s level taught during winter 2022/23. Experiences document that students are interested in communicating scientific findings, and eager to develop their skills for this important task. Course structure, experiences and outcomes are documented, and slides as well as science communication outputs are shared via an open science platform.

Statistics

loading

References

ALLCOTT, H., & GENTZKOW, M. 2017. “Social Media and Fake News in the 2016 Election“, in: Journal of Economic Perspectives, 31:2, S. 211–236.

BADDELEY, A. 2010. “Working memory“, in: Current Biology, 20:4, S. 136–140.

BIGGS, J. 1996. “Enhancing teaching through constructive alignment“, in: Higher Education 32, S. 347–364.

BIGGS, J., & TANG, C. 2011. Teaching for quality learning at university. Open University Press/Mc Graw-Hill Education.

BLOOM, B. S. (Hg). 1973. Taxonomie von Lernzielen im kognitiven Bereich. 3. Auflage, Beltz.

COWAN, N. (2010). “The Magical Mystery Four: How is Working Memory Capacity Limited, and Why?“, in: Current directions in psychological science, 19:1, S. 51–57.

CLARK, J., & PAIVIO, A. 1991. “Dual coding theory and education“, in: Educational Psychology Review, 3:3, S. 149–210.

HOCKLEY, W. 2008. “The picture superiority effect in associative recognition“, in: Memory & Cognition, 36:7, S. 1351-1359.

LAND BADEN-WÜRTTEMBERG (Hg.). 2020. „Sandwichprinzip.“ (https://lehrerfortbildung-bw.de/u_gestaltlehrlern/projekte/sol/fb1/03_grundlagen/sandwichprinzip/; Zugriff: 19.06.2023).

NIEMANN, P., SCHRÖGEL, P., & HAUSER, C. 2017. „Präsentationsformen der externen Wissenschaftskommunikation: Ein Vorschlag zur Typologisierung.“, in: Zeitschrift für Angewandte Linguistik, 67:1, S. 81–113.

NUISSL, E., & SIEBERT, H. 2013. Lehren an der VHS. Bielefeld: W. Bertelsmann.

RYAN, R., & DECI, E. 2000. “Self-Determination Theory and the Facilitation of Intrinsic Motivation, Social Development, and Well-Being“, in: American Psychologist, 55:1, S. 68–78.

SWELLER, J. 2005. “Implications of cognitive load theory for multimedia learning.“, in: The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning 3.2, S. 19-30.

SILVA, J., & BULTITUDE, K. 2009. “Best practice in communications training for public engagement with science, technology, engineering and mathematics”, in: Journal of Science Communication, 8:2, A03.

VAN MERRIENBOER, J., & SWELLER, J. 2005. “Cognitive Load Theory and Complex Learning: Recent Developments and Future Directions“, in: Educational Psychology Review, 17, S. 147–177.

Published
2023-12-20
Language
English
Type, method or approach
text
Keywords
Science Communication, Psychology, Course Development